The Sensitivity–Security Link in Early Childhood
Maternal Sensitivity and Child Secure Base Use in Early
Childhood: Studies in Different Cultural Contexts
German Posada Purdue University
Jill Trumbell University of New Hampshire
Magaly Noblega Pontificia Universidad Cat�olica del Per�u
Sandra Plata, Paola Pe~na, and Olga A. Carbonell
Pontificia Universidad Javeriana
Ting Lu Auburn University
This study tested whether maternal sensitivity and child
security are related during early childhood and whether such an
association is found in different cultural and social contexts.
Mother–child dyads (N = 237) from four different countries
(Colombia, Mexico, Peru, and the United States) were observed in
naturalistic settings when children were between 36 and 72 months
of age. Maternal and child behavior during interac- tions at home
and in the playground were described using Q methodology. Findings
reveal that across cul- tures, concurrent maternal sensitivity and
more specific behavioral domains of maternal care (e.g.,
contributions to harmonious interactions and secure base support)
are important for children’s attachment security during early
childhood. Implications for the study of attachment relationships
beyond infancy and in diverse contexts are highlighted.
Bowlby (1969/1982, 1988) argued that attachment relationships
are a life-span phenomenon that plays a significant role in
development and how we con- ceive of, feel, and behave in close
relationships. Child–mother relationships are considered central
during the first years of life. The relation between quality of
care, that is, sensitivity, and child attach- ment security is a
cornerstone of the Bowlby–Ains- worth perspective. Indeed, a great
deal of research has supported the link between maternal
sensitivity and attachment security during infancy; less is known
about those relationships and the patterning of maternal and child
behavior during interactions in the preschool years. Theoretical
elaboration beyond infancy, for example, childhood, is incipient as
questions regarding the developmental course of such relationships
are being addressed in research. Studying child–mother
relationships in contexts
where they are constructed, maintained, and elabo- rated
during the preschool years is an important step in that direction.
This study addresses the hypothesis about the relation between
maternal sen- sitivity and child security during early childhood.
Bowlby was cognizant of the many contextual and cultural
variations in child–mother attachment relationships. Amid those
variations, he also saw commonalities in the way children and
mothers interact with each other and organize their behavior during
interactions (Bowlby, 1969/1982). The uni- versality hypotheses
derived from attachment the- ory have been questioned in view of
the diversity of ways child–mother interactions transpire in dif-
ferent cultural and social contexts (e.g., LeVine & Norman,
2001; Rothbaum & Morelli, 2005; Roth- baum, Weisz, Pott,
Miyake, & Morelli, 2001). Empirical research explicitly testing
the universality of child–mother attachment relationships is scant.
The few existing studies lend initial support to theSome of the
ideas and data presented were supported by
grants from the National Science Foundation (BCS-0645530),
the Kinley Trust, and the Purdue Research Foundation to the first
author.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
German Posada, Department of Human Development & Family
Studies, Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana 47907. Elec-
tronic mail may be sent to [email protected]
© 2015 The Authors Child Development © 2015 Society for
Research in Child Development, Inc. All rights reserved.
0009-3920/2016/8701-0024 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12454
Child Development, January/February 2016, Volume 87, Number
1, Pages 297–311
universality of key notions (see below), as well as to the
idea that attachment relationships during infancy are sensitive to
the characteristics of the ecological settings in which those
relationships develop. Empirical evidence testing the universality
of attachment relationships beyond infancy and into early childhood
is even scarcer. In this study, we specifically examine the notion
that the organiza- tion of maternal caregiving behavior (i.e.,
sensitiv- ity) is significantly related to the organization of
preschoolers’ secure base behavior (i.e., security) both in a U.S.
sample and in samples from cultural backgrounds different from that
of Western indus- trialized societies where the phenomenon is
typically studied.
The Sensitivity–Security Link in Early Childhood
Foundational research on the relation between sensitivity and
attachment security was conducted by Ainsworth. Her landmark
studies in Uganda and Baltimore indicated that a mother’s
awareness, cor- rect interpretation, promptness, and
appropriateness of response to her infant’s signals and communica-
tions were significantly associated with an infant’s trust in his
mother’s availability and response (Ains- worth, Blehar, Waters,
& Wall, 1978). Her Baltimore study revealed that maternal
sensitivity was signifi- cantly related to attachment security.
Infants who effectively used their mothers as a secure base tended
to have mothers deemed to be sensitive. Although the robustness of
the sensitivity–security association reported in studies conducted
after Ains- worth’s is not as strong as the one she found, research
has, for the most part, confirmed her find- ings during infancy—see
De Wolff and van IJzen- doorn’s (1997) meta-analytic study.
Furthermore, intervention studies aimed at improving parental
sensitivity provide evidence of a causal link between sensitivity
and security, with enhanced sensitive caregiving leading to
increased infant security (Bak- ermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn,
& Juffer, 2003).
Although a child’s ability to use his mother as a secure base
is put together during the first year, it requires continuous
support during child–mother interactions in early childhood.
Consolidation and elaboration of secure base use beyond infancy
entails concurrent and developmentally appropriate secure base
support. Thus, some researchers have emphasized the importance of
maternal input for children’s security outcomes past infancy (e.g.,
Ainsworth, 1991; Bowlby, 1969/1982; Waters, Posada, Crowell, &
Lay, 1994) and hypothesize that quality of care continues to play a
key role in
attachment relationships. Evidence supporting the
sensitivity–security link in early childhood is, how- ever,
relatively scant. Furthermore, research that characterizes maternal
secure base support at this age, beyond global assessments of
sensitivity and their links with security, is needed to detail the
developmental course of these relationships.
Empirical support for the sensitivity–security link in early
childhood is crucial to substantiate claims from attachment theory
about the importance of concurrent and continuous support of
child–mother attachment relationships (e.g., Pianta, Sroufe, &
Egeland, 1989; Sroufe, 1988; Waters et al., 1994). Moreover, it
would validate the notion that although significant, experiences
during infancy alone do not determine later outcomes, and that a
developmental analysis is necessary (Bowlby, 1969/ 1982; Sroufe,
2002; Vereijken, Riksen-Walraven, & Kondo-Ikemura, 1997).
Findings from existing stud- ies seem to indicate that sensitivity
and security are indeed associated during the preschool years
(e.g., Barnett, Kidwell, & Leung, 1998; George & Solo- mon,
2008; Stevenson-Hinde & Shouldice, 1995). The majority of these
studies show that summary ratings of maternal sensitivity are
significantly related to children’s attachment security. However,
they do not specify maternal caregiving behavior and its
organization that may account for the asso- ciations reported
between global ratings of sensitiv- ity and child security. As
such, maternal support relevant for secure base behavior during
early childhood remains unclear.
To elaborate the construct of quality of care beyond infancy,
Posada and colleagues (Posada, Kaloustian, Richmond, & Moreno,
2007) assembled a developmentally appropriate Q-sort to assess
maternal sensitivity and studied four specific domains of maternal
behavior during early child- hood: behavior that contributes to
harmonious child–mother interactions, provision of secure base
support, supervision, and maternal consideration of the child’s
perspective when setting limits. The notion that maternal behavior
that promotes harmonious interactions with their children is at the
center of secure attachment relationships was a key outcome of
Ainsworth’s naturalistic study in Balti- more (Ainsworth, Bell,
& Stayton, 1971; Ainsworth et al., 1978). A mother’s ability to
participate and contribute to positive exchanges is at the heart of
secure relationships. Findings reported by Posada et al. (2007)
support this idea. Furthermore, provi- sion of appropriate secure
base support is in all likeli- hood related to consolidation and
skillful use of mother as a secure base from which to explore and
298 Posada et al.
to which to retreat. That is, supporting children’s
explorations and providing a haven of safety are considered
essential for the organization of secure base behavior (Marvin
& Britner, 1999; Waters & Cummings, 2000; Waters,
Kondo-Ikemura, Posada, & Richters, 1991). Similarly,
supervision or monitor- ing of preschoolers’ whereabouts has been
suggested an important aspect of maternal behavior during
childhood. Mothers’ ability to keep track of her child, anticipate
problematic situations, and intervene when necessary are all
considered impor- tant aspects of sensitive maternal behavior
(George & Solomon, 2008; Posada et al., 2007; Waters et al.,
1991). Finally, the provision of limits and bound- aries around a
child’s activities becomes a salient issue in child–mother
relationships beyond infancy (Ainsworth, Bell, & Stayton,
1974). Sensitively responding to a child’s communications involves
a consideration of the child’s needs and wants even when setting
limits and boundaries. Findings from two studies (Posada et al.,
2007) indicated that in addition to a general index of sensitivity,
all domains, but limit setting, were significantly associ- ated
with child security. The evidence for limit set- ting was mixed.
Although thought provoking, those findings are preliminary and need
to be explored further with different samples.
Thus, we tested the hypothesis that maternal sensitivity and
the organization of children’s secure base behavior (as indexed by
a security score) dur- ing the preschool years are significantly
associated. Additionally, we investigated the specific aspects of
maternal behavior in interactions with her child mentioned above
and expected contributions to harmonious interactions, secure base
support, and supervision to be significantly related to preschool-
ers’ security. On the basis of previous findings, we explored
whether sensitive consideration of the child’s needs and wants in
setting limits and boundaries was also associated with child
security.
The Sensitivity–Security Link in Diverse Cultural Contexts
Bowlby (1958, 1969/1982) conceptualized child attachment and
corresponding maternal caregiving behavior as species
characteristic adaptations. His rationale, in accordance with
modern evolutionary theory, posits the attachment and caregiving
sys- tems as products of the natural selection process leading to
human evolution. These complementary systems, he argued, were
selected for the survival advantages they afforded to those
children who sought and maintained proximity and contact with
their caregivers. He suggested that in the context of
everyday interactions, infants organize an attach- ment behavioral
system with the goal of maintain- ing proximity to those who care
for them. Similarly, he argued that maternal caregiving is
organized as a behavioral system. As evolutionary products, these
systems are expected to be observable in most humans as long as the
environ- ment they inhabit is within the range of ecological
conditions in which they evolved. This is not to say that patterns
of behavior exhibited are stereotyped. Bowlby and Ainsworth were
well aware of the existing variability in children’s and mothers’
behavior across context and culture (e.g., Ains- worth, 1977;
Bowlby, 1969/1982). Despite this vari- ability, they argued that
patterns of behavior that result in the care of infants and in the
attachment of children to parents can be discerned in almost all
humans. Furthermore, they maintained that for all child–mother
dyads, the overall organization of a child’s attachment behavior is
interlocked with the organization of her or his mother’s caregiving
behavior.
Accordingly, the aforementioned association between maternal
sensitivity and child security is expected to be significant in
different social and cul- tural contexts. However, explicit
empirical evalua- tion of this hypothesis during infancy is limited
and practically nonexistent during the early child- hood years
(see, Posada et al., 1999; van IJzendoorn & Sagi-Schwartz,
2008). To be clear, the numerous studies supporting the association
between mater- nal sensitivity and infant security offer the
impres- sion that the universality of the link has been proven. A
careful look at the existing literature, however, shows a different
picture as most research has been conducted in Western
industrialized coun- tries (e.g., De Wolff & van IJzendoorn,
1997). A more recent review of studies conducted in several
countries in Africa, China, Israel, and Japan (van IJzendoorn &
Sagi-Schwartz, 2008) underscores the need to further investigate
the issue. Research is not only scarce, but assessments of
sensitivity in many cases are also limited. Specifically, of the 16
reports cited by can IJzendoorn and Sagi-Schwartz (2008), only 4,
other than the 1 by Ainsworth in Uganda, assessed sensitivity
directly. One of these studies, conducted in Japan, did not find
maternal sensitivity significantly associated with infant secu-
rity (Nakagawa, Lamb, & Miyaki, 1992), yet as van IJzendoorn
and Sagi-Schwartz (2008) discussed, this study is difficult to
evaluate. In the other studies cited, reported assessments of
maternal sensitivity were indirect at best. For example, in a study
Sensitivity–Security Link 299
conducted on the Gusii society in Kenya (Kermoian &
Leiderman, 1986), sensitivity was inferred via mothers’ age,
household size, and the birth of a new infant, or in a different
study by Hu and Meng conducted in China (1996; cited by van
IJzendoorn & Sagi-Schwartz, 2008), sensitivity was indirectly
assessed through maternal involvement in the care of their infants.
van IJzendoorn and Sagi-Schwartz concluded that more cross-cultural
studies are required to settle the issue.
A Cross-Country Comparison
Cross-context studies including samples from societies
different from those in industrialized coun- tries offer an
opportunity to falsify central notions in attachment theory and to
test the validity of core hypotheses in contexts different from
those in which they were developed. Latin American coun- tries
present a good arena to conduct such tests. Countries in Latin
America include cultures with some characteristics different from
those of the Anglo-American culture. Their social structure,
including family organization, is largely a legacy of Spanish
settlers and colonizers (Valenzuela, 1997). Latino families have
been described as more socially oriented rather than
individualistic, with extended family as a very important source of
social contact and support (Leyendecker & Lamb, 1999; Martin
& Colbert, 1997). Furthermore, they tend to hold childrearing
attitudes that emphasize interdependence rather than independence
(Leyen- decker, Lamb, Scholmerich, & Fracasso, 1995). Thus,
Latino families seem to present a different case from that of
Anglo-Saxon, North American, middle-class families. The
characteristics just men- tioned and, apparently, different
conceptions and beliefs about family relations might influence
child– mother interactions. Indeed, studies have found parental
beliefs linked to parenting behavior (e.g., Leadbeater &
Bishop, 1994; Okagaki & Johnson- Divecha, 1993; Zuniga, 1992).
Families from different Latin American countries and social
groups cannot, however, be character- ized as the same; in fact,
their heterogeneity has been shown to be associated with different
develop- mental outcomes (Rivera et al., 2008). Thus, in test- ing
the generality of core attachment hypotheses, it is important to
use samples from different countries and social contexts to better
capture such variabil- ity. The studies presented test the notions
that maternal sensitivity and child secure base use are
significantly associated during early childhood and that such an
association is found in different cul-
tural and social contexts. We took advantage of existing
collaborations from different research groups and investigated the
associations between maternal sensitivity and child security in
samples from Colombia, Mexican immigrants to the United States,
Peru, and the United States by observing maternal and preschoolers’
behavior during child– mother interactions in naturalistic settings
at home and playgrounds.
On the basis of the predictions from the theory and initial
evidence, we expected a general index of quality of care (i.e.,
maternal sensitivity) to be signif- icantly and positively related
to children’s security in naturalistic settings across all the
samples. Also, we investigated the associations between specific
domains of maternal behavior (i.e., contributions to harmonious
interactions, secure base support, super- vision, and sensitive
consideration of the child’s needs and wants in setting limits and
boundaries) and preschoolers’ security in each sample.
Method
Participants
A total of 237 mother–child dyads across four samples from
different countries were observed at their homes and at parks.
Specifically, 85 Colom- bian, 46 Mexican immigrants to the United
States, 30 Peruvian, and 76 U.S. dyads participated. While these
were samples of convenience recruited inde- pendently for studies
with unique questions, data- collection techniques were similar
enough (though not identical) to provide an ideal opportunity for
cross-country comparisons regarding the sensitiv- ity–security link
during early childhood. They rep- resent diverse cultural groups
from the Western hemisphere; specifically, each varies in the
extent to which they encompass individualist versus collec- tivist
values (Oyserman, Coon, & Kemmelmeier, 2002), with the
potential to impact parenting prac- tices (Rudy & Grusec,
2006).
Within each culture, samples were recruited through local
child-care centers, preschools, and community fliers. Colombian
dyads came from sociodemographic Sectors 2, 3, and 4 (of the six
sec- tors) in Bogot�a; they were visited between 2008 and 2011.
Mexican immigrant dyads came primarily from a low socioeconomic
background and were recruited in a Midwestern university town
between 2007 and 2009. Dyads in Peru were recruited between 2011
and 2012 from a working-class neigh- borhood southeast of Lima. The
U.S. child–mother dyads were recruited between 2003 and 2006 from
300 Posada et al.
a Midwestern university town and came from a middle-class
background; 79% of the U.S. sample was non-Hispanic Caucasian. All
dyads from Colombia, Mexican immigrants, and Peru were His- panic.
Overall, children were between 36 and 72 months of age (M = 50.3);
means across coun- tries ranged from 42.9 to 60.7 months. All
children were healthy and came from nonclinical popula- tions.
There were 121 boys and 116 girls. Mothers were between 19 and 49
years old; the range of average maternal age across countries was
29.8– 35.0 years. Mothers’ years of education ranged from 1 to 24,
with 1 equivalent to a year of primary schooling and 24
corresponding to a doctoral degree; mean years of maternal
education across countries ranged from 9.5 to 16.5. Most (146)
moth- ers worked outside the home; there was no employ- ment
information available for 2 Colombian mothers. Table 1 presents a
summary of demo- graphic information by sample.
Procedures
Information about maternal behavior and child secure base
behavior was collected at home and in the playground. The study was
explained in detail to mothers who indicated interest. If they con-
sented, a home or a playground visit was sched- uled. Home visits
lasted about 2.5 hr for all samples except Peru, for which home
visits lasted about 1 hr. Playground visits lasted between 60 and
75 min for all samples except the Mexican immigrant sample for
which playground visits lasted about 2 hr. In the sample of Mexican
immigrants, two visits per setting were conducted
—maternal behavior was observed in one visit and child
behavior in the other. In Colombia, two play- ground visits were
conducted, with one to observe maternal behavior and one to observe
child behav- ior. In all other cases (home observations in Colom-
bia, Peru, and the United States, as well as playground
observations for Peru and the United States) descriptions of
maternal and child behavior were obtained from the same visit. In
all samples, observers who described maternal behavior were
independent from those who described child behav- ior; in no case
did an observer describe both mother and child behavior for both
members of a dyad. Observers’ descriptions of maternal and child
behavior were aggregated both within and across settings. All
mothers also provided sociodemo- graphic information including
their age, ethnicity, education, and job within the last 3 years.
Assessment
The Maternal Behavior for Preschoolers Q-Set (MBPQS; Posada
et al., 2007) and Attachment Q-Set (AQS; Waters, 1995) were used to
describe mothers’ and children’s behavior, respectively. Home
visits included some structured activities and other research tasks
that varied from sample to sample; specifically, in some samples
mothers were asked to have a snack, read a book, construct a
puzzle, and/ or play with their child. Home visits also had an
unstructured part, such that mothers were told to go about their
activities as they would normally. In all samples, park visits were
unstructured, with mothers and children going about their regular
activities. Observers were allowed to interact naturally with
Table 1 Demographic Characteristics
Colombia Mexico Peru United States
N 85 46 30 76 Children Gender: boy/girl 41/44 28/18 17/13
35/41 Age in months (SD) 42.9 (2.1)a 55.5 (6.4)b 60.7 (7.9)c 51.5
(7.9)d Range 39–48 46–66 45–72 36–67
Mothers Age in years (SD) 31.2 (6.4)a 29.8 (4.8)a 35.0 (5.7)b
34.3 (5.8)b Range 19–45 21–41 25–45 22–49
Education in years (SD) 13.4 (3.8)a 9.5 (3.3)b 15.3 (1.5)c
16.5 (2.4)c Range 5–22 1–16 12–16 9–24
Occupation: home/out of home 26/57 24/22 10/20 29/47
Note. Information on maternal education in Peru was gathered
as having completed high school (12 years) or having completed
under- graduate studies (16 years). For child age maternal age, and
maternal education, means in the same row that do not share
subscripts differ at p < .05.
Sensitivity–Security Link 301
mother and child at home and park. After finishing a visit,
observers returned to the laboratory and inde- pendently described
maternal behavior and child behavior with the MBPQS and the AQS,
respectively.
Mother and child behavior were reported on by observers
(authors and trained graduate and under- graduate students) who
were native and lived in the same country as the participants they
observed, and largely matched participant ethnicity, except in
cases for which no matches were available. Impor- tantly, however,
observers interacted and spoke flu- ently with participants in
their native language (Spanish-speaking observers visited
Spanish-speak- ing families, while English-speaking observers vis-
ited English-speaking families). Observers were trained in the use
of both Q-sets. Training for each Q-set consisted of first reading
and discussing the meaning of the items. This was followed by three
to six practice observations and q-descriptions of maternal and
child behavior during live or video- taped child–mother
interactions at home. Trainees’ descriptions were compared to those
of an expert; an observer was considered trained when she or he
obtained an interobserver reliability with an expert (i.e.,
correlation corrected for number of observers using the
Spearman–Brown formula) of at least 0.70 in three practice
observations.
Maternal Behavior
Maternal behavior during interactions with their children was
described with the MBPQS (Posada et al., 2007). This Q-set assesses
the organization of maternal secure base support in naturalistic
set- tings. The MBPQS has 90 items that describe age- relevant
caregiving behavior. It provides an overall summary index of the
quality of care, that is, maternal sensitivity, and scores on
age-salient domains of behavior concerned with maternal con-
tributions to harmonious interactions with her child, provision of
secure base support, supervision, and limit setting. Empirical
support for the reliabil- ity and validity of the MBPQS and the
behavioral domains has been found in two studies (Posada et al.,
2007; Richmond, Posada, & Jacobs, 2001). Members of the
research teams working with the Colombian, Mexican, and Peruvian
samples first translated the MBPQS from English into Spanish.
Research teams adapted the MBPQS to the particu- lar Spanish
terminology characteristic of their samples. The different versions
were then back- translated by a different researcher. The English
versions were compared to the original version and items were
revised if their meaning was inaccurate.
The MBPQS was completed after each visit by one or two
observers who sorted the items along a continuum from least
characteristic to most character- istic using a distribution of
nine piles with 10 items each. Following Q methodology, each
observer ini- tially divided the 90 items into three piles: charac-
teristic, neither characteristic nor uncharacteristic, or
uncharacteristic. The three piles were then sub- divided into nine
piles of 10 items each, ranging from 1 (most uncharacteristic) to 9
(most characteris- tic). The pile number in which an item was
placed is the rating for that item. Maternal behavior was described
by two observers in 125 of the 169 visits in Colombia, 42 of the 92
visits in the Mexican sam- ple, 25 of the 60 visits in Peru, and in
47 of the 151 visits in the United States. While attempts were made
to have completely independent observers report on maternal
behavior across contexts (e.g., different observers for maternal
behavior at the park and in the home) and this was the case for
many families visited, it was not always feasible given constraints
of observers’ and participants’ schedules. As mentioned before,
however, in no case did observers of maternal behavior describe
child behavior for the same dyad.
Mean interobserver reliability indices (calculated from the
agreement between q-descriptions from independent observers; Block,
1978) by sample were Colombian, 0.85 (range = 0.67–0.96); Mexican
immigrants, 0.82 (range = 0.61–0.94); Peru, 0.81 (range =
0.62–0.93); and U.S., 0.86 (range = 0.67– 0.95). The descriptions
provided by observers were averaged into a Q-composite description
for each setting (home and park). Descriptions of maternal behavior
for six Mexican and five Peruvian partici- pants had low
interobserver reliability (i.e., < 0.60) for one of the visits;
those observations were not included when computing sensitivity
scores. For each mother, sensitivity scores were computed for the
home, park, and overall composite (aggregated home and park) by
correlating each respective q- description with the sensitivity
criterion sort that describes the prototypically sensitive mother.
A sen- sitivity score expresses the degree of correspon- dence
(i.e., correlation) between a mother’s description and the MBPQS
criterion sort (Posada et al., 2007).
Scale scores were also calculated for each setting by
averaging each scale’s corresponding item scores. The domain
maternal contributions to harmo- nious interactions with her child
consists of 21 items (a = .93, overall sample) that refer to both
behav- ioral and affective involvement in the exchanges of a mother
with her child (e.g., “Interactions appro-
302 Posada et al.
priately vigorous and exciting as judged from child’s
response” and “Mother behaves as part of a team, exchanges with
child are harmonious”). Pro- vision of secure base support (23
items; a = .91, over- all sample) assesses a mother’s provision of
a haven of safety and support of exploration (e.g., “When child
returns to her, mother is unresponsive or business like in
acknowledging child’s return” [re- versed] and “Smoothly
facilitates explorations away and returns to her”). Supervision
consists of nine items (a = .80, overall sample) that refer to
keeping track and monitoring her child (e.g., “Is two steps ahead
of child, anticipates potential conflictive situ- ations and does
something to prevent escalation”). Sensitive limit setting (six
items except for the Colombian sample for which an item had a nega-
tive association with the overall scale and thus the item was
excluded in calculating the scale; a = .67, overall sample) taps
into a mother’s consideration of her child’s desires and wants when
setting limits (e.g., “In limit setting, mother negotiates with
child until a mutually satisfying solution is achieved”).
Cronbach’s alphas for the full sample as well as for each subsample
are presented in Table 2. Also, the
correlations among scale scores as well as the corre- lations
between each scale and the overall compos- ite sensitivity score
for the full sample and each subsample are presented in Table 2.
Secure Base Behavior
Children’s behavior during interactions with their mothers
was described with the AQS (Waters, 1995). This instrument has 90
items and assesses the organization of infants’ and preschool
children’s attachment behavior in naturalistic settings; its
validity has been documented elsewhere (e.g., Ped- erson &
Moran, 1996; van IJzendoorn, Vereijken, Bakermans-Kranenburg, &
Riksen-Walraven, 2004; Vaughn & Waters, 1990). The validity of
the AQS for use in cultures other than those representing Western
industrialized countries has also been sup- ported in studies
conducted in China, Colombia, Israel, Japan, Peru, and Taiwan
(e.g., Posada et al., 1995; Posada et al., 2013; van IJzendoorn et
al., 2004; Vereijken et al., 1997).
As in the previous case, members of the different research
teams translated the AQS from English
Table 2 Composite Maternal Behavior for Preschoolers Q-Set
Intercorrelations and Cronbach’s Alphas
1 2 3 4 Sensitivity
Overall sample 1. Smooth interactions (a = .93) — .94* 2.
Secure base support (a = .91) .91* — .92* 3. Supervision (a = .80)
.73* .70* — .78* 4. Limit setting (a = .67) .51* .47* .46* — .55*
Colombia 1. Smooth interactions (a = .90) — .94* 2. Secure
base support (a = .89) .85* — .90* 3. Supervision (a = .82) .79*
.67* — .83* 4. Limit setting (a = .52) .36* .21* .42* — .31**
Mexico 1. Smooth interactions (a = .95) — .95* 2. Secure base
support (a = .93) .93* — .95* 3. Supervision (a = .62) .57* .60* —
.70* 4. Limit setting (a = .75) .64* .63* .63* — .71*
Peru 1. Smooth interactions (a = .96) — .95* 2. Secure base
support (a = .94) .94* — .93* 3. Supervision (a = .83) .78* .76* —
.89* 4. Limit setting (a = .62) .50** .51** .48** — .60**
United States 1. Smooth interactions (a = .87) — .87* 2.
Secure base support (a = .86) .89* — .89* 3. Supervision (a = .62)
.56* .57* — .61* 4. Limit setting (a = .71) .35** .33** .15 —
.43***
*p ≤ .05. **p ≤ .01. ***p ≤ .001, one-tail tests.
Sensitivity–Security Link 303
into Spanish. The different versions were then translated
back into English by a different researcher. The English versions
were compared to the original version and items were revised if
neces- sary. The AQS was completed after each home/ park visit by
one or two observers who sorted the items along a continuum from
least characteristic to most characteristic using a distribution of
nine piles with 10 items each. Observers followed the same
procedures described for the MBPQS. Child behav- ior was described
by two observers in 128 of the 169 visits in Colombia, 88 of the 92
visits in the Mexican sample, 28 of the 60 visits in Peru, and in
111 of the 151 visits in the United States. As in the previous
case, attempts were made to have com- pletely independent observers
report on child behavior across contexts and, although this was the
case for many families visited, it was not always feasible.
Mean interobserver reliability indices (calculated from the
agreement between q-descriptions from independent observers) by
sample were Colombian, 0.84 (range = 0.64–0.95); Mexican, 0.80
(range = 0.63–0.93); Peru, 0.79 (range = 0.64–0.94); and U.S., 0.78
(range = 0.60–0.92). The descriptions provided by observers were
averaged into a Q-composite description for each setting.
Descriptions of child behavior for 1 Colombian and 11 U.S.
participants had low interobserver reliability (< 0.60) for one
of the visits; those observations were not included
when computing security scores. Home, park, and overall
composite (aggregated park and home) security scores were
calculated for each child by correlating each respective
q-description with the security criterion sort that describes the
prototypi- cally secure child (Waters, 1995). A security score
expresses the degree of correspondence between a child’s
description and the AQS criterion sort. The Pearson correlation
coefficient between those two descriptions is the security score.
Results
Preliminary analyses revealed significant subsample
differences in demographic variables (see Table 1). Specifically,
differences in child age were significant for all subsample
comparisons. Also, analyses of maternal age indicated that
Colombian and Mexi- can mothers were significantly younger than
Peru- vian and U.S. mothers. Finally, maternal education
significantly differed among the subsamples. Mexi- can immigrant
and Colombian mothers reported significantly fewer years of
education than Peruvian and U.S. mothers; also, Mexican mothers
reported significantly fewer years of education than Colom- bian
mothers. Mean composite scores, standard deviations, and range of
sensitivity and security scores for each sample are presented in
Table 3. The mean sensitivity score for the overall composite
Table 3 Descriptive Statistics for Maternal Sensitivity and
Child Security
Overall sample Colombia Mexico Peru United States
Sensitivity composite M (SD) 0.51 (0.32) 0.48 (0.29)a 0.46
(0.35)ab 0.30 (0.44)b 0.67 (0.20)c Range �0.67 to 0.86 �0.35 to
0.84 �0.65 to 0.80 �0.67 to 0.79 �0.33 to 0.86
Sensitivity home M (SD) 0.43 (0.34) 0.43 (0.29)a 0.34 (0.38)a
0.12 (0.43)b 0.60 (0.20)c Range �0.55 to 0.82 �0.50 to 0.80 �0.53
to 0.77 �0.55 to 0.71 �0.25 to 0.82
Sensitivity park M (SD) 0.51 (0.32) 0.43 (0.32)a 0.50
(0.32)ab 0.46 (0.41)ab 0.63 (0.23)b Range �0.62 to 0.84 �0.31 to
0.82 �0.61 to 0.79 �0.62 to 0.78 �0.30 to 0.84
Security composite M (SD) 0.39 (0.22) 0.34 (0.22)a 0.40
(0.20)ab 0.31 (0.26)a 0.49 (0.18)b Range �0.35 to 0.73 �0.25 to
0.69 �0.07 to 0.68 �0.35 to 0.68 �0.14 to 0.73
Security home M (SD) 0.34 (0.23) 0.27 (0.24)a 0.38 (0.20)bc
0.27 (0.25)ab 0.43 (0.19)c Range �0.38 to 0.75 �0.22 to 0.67 �0.15
to 0.64 �0.38 to 0.65 �0.16 to 0.75
Security park M (SD) 0.34 (0.23) 0.30 (0.19)a 0.33 (0.22)a
0.26 (0.30)a 0.43 (0.20)b Range �0.57 to 0.71 �0.29 to 0.67 �0.14
to 0.66 �0.57 to 0.63 �0.23 to 0.71
Note. Means in the same row that do not share subscripts
differ at p < .05.
304 Posada et al.
description (home and park) for the entire sample was 0.51
(SD = 0.32), with scores that ranged from �0.67 to 0.86. The mean
sensitivity score at home was 0.43 (range = �0.55 to 0.82, SD =
0.34) and at the park was 0.51 (range = �0.62 to 0.84, SD = 0.32).
The mean security score for the com- posite description was 0.39
(SD = 0.22), with scores that ranged from �0.35 to 0.73. The mean
security score for child behavior at home was 0.34 (range = �0.38
to 0.75, SD = 0.23) and at the park was 0.34 (range = �0.57 to
0.71, SD = 0.23). One- way analyses of variance revealed
significant sample differences in the means for both overall
sensitivity and security composites. Post hoc com- parisons
indicated that the U.S. mothers obtained significantly higher
sensitivity scores than mothers in any other sample. Also,
Colombian mothers had significantly higher sensitivity scores than
Peruvian mothers. In addition, post hoc analyses revealed that
children in the United States had significantly higher security
scores than Colombian and Peru- vian children (see Table 3).
Correlation analyses investigating the associa- tions between
demographic information (i.e., moth- ers’ age, education, and
occupation and child age and gender) and sensitivity and security
composites for the overall sample showed that maternal age and
education were significantly related to maternal sensitivity. Older
and more educated mothers received higher sensitivity scores (r =
.14, p < .05; r = .21, p < .01, respectively). Analyses by
sample indicated two significant associations; specifically,
maternal education and child age were significantly associated with
sensitivity in the United States (r = .25, p < .05; and r =
�.26, p < .05, respectively). Mothers with more years of
education and younger children obtained higher sensitivity scores.
As secu- rity is concerned, analyses for the overall sample showed
that no demographic variable was signifi- cantly associated with
child security. Analyses by country indicated that child security
was negatively correlated with maternal age in Colombia (r = �.22,
p < .05). Child security was found to be associated with child
gender in the sample of Mexican immi-
grants, with girls obtaining higher security scores than boys
(r = .45, p < .01). No other significant associations were
found. Based on these results, demographic variables significantly
related to sensi- tivity and/or security (i.e., maternal age and
educa- tion, and child age and gender) were used as covariates in
all subsequent analysis.
Analyses indicated that observations of maternal sensitivity
at home and park were significantly related for the total sample (r
= .51, p < .001) and for each of the samples (Table 4); similar
results were found for observations of secure base behav- ior in
both settings (r = .51, p < .001, for the total sample). To test
whether maternal sensitivity and child security scores were
significantly associated, we conducted partial correlation
analyses. We stud- ied the relation between the constructs when
each was assessed in an independent setting and at a different time
from the other; that is, we considered the associations between
sensitivity at home and child secure base use at the park, and
between sen- sitivity at the park and child secure base use at
home. The resultant correlation coefficients for each cross-setting
analysis were converted to Fisher’s z, averaged, and then converted
back to a correlation coefficient. Results indicated that even when
they are assessed on different times and settings, sensitivity and
security were positively and signifi- cantly related for the total
sample (r = .36, p < .001) and for each subsample studied (Table
4).
Next, we studied whether the domains of care- giving behavior
were significantly associated with the organization of child secure
base behavior. Again, we investigated the association between the
domains and security when each was assessed in an independent
setting and at a different time from the other; that is, we
considered the relations between each domain at home and child
secure base use at the park, and between each domain at the park
and child secure base use at home. The correlation coefficients for
each cross-setting analy- sis were converted to Fisher’s z,
averaged, and then converted back to a correlation coefficient.
Partial correlations controlling for maternal age, education,
Table 4 Partial Correlations Between Maternal Sensitivity and
Child Security
Overall sample Colombia Mexico Peru United States
Sensitivity home—Sensitivity park .51* .54* .61** .38* .54*
Security home—Security park .51* .52* .41 .55** .36**
Sensitivity—Security across settings .36* .31 .30* .43** .23*
Note. Maternal age education, child age, and gender were
controlled for in all analyses. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p
< .001, one-tail tests.
Sensitivity–Security Link 305
and child age and gender showed each domain to be
significantly related to secure base behavior in the overall
sample. Specifically, child security was positively and
significantly related to maternal con- tribution to harmonious
interactions (r = .36, p < .001), secure base support (r = .31,
p < .001), supervision/monitoring (r = .29, p < .001), and
sen- sitive limit setting (r = .21, p < .001). Associations by
sample are presented in Table 5.
Finally, we examined the extent to which associ- ations
between maternal sensitivity and child secu- rity differed by
cultural context. Moderation analyses confirmed that while there
were between- country differences in security scores (as noted
above), no Culture 9 Sensitivity interaction pre- dicted security.
That is, the sensitivity–security association did not significantly
differ across coun- tries; this was true for the cross-context
associations both at the level of the sensitivity composites by
context (i.e., home and park) and the level of mater- nal
caregiving domains.
Discussion
The notion that maternal sensitivity plays a key role in the
organization of a child’s secure base behavior is central to
attachment theory. Although the link between sensitivity and
security has been clearly established during infancy, research on
the associa- tions between the constructs in naturalistic settings
during early childhood is rare. This is an important developmental
inquiry that needs to be addressed and is thus a primary goal of
the current study.
Before discussing our main findings, we note that maternal
age and education were found to be significantly and positively
related to sensitivity for the entire sample. For the subsamples,
however, only the association between maternal education and
sensitivity was significant in the United States. The more years of
education mothers reported, the more sensitive their caregiving.
This finding is con- sistent with those reported for mothers of
infants (e.g., Pederson & Moran, 1996). Child security was not
significantly associated with any of the demo- graphic variables
for the overall sample. For the subsamples, however, younger
mothers had more secure children in the Colombian sample, and a
gender-related association was found in the sample of Mexican
immigrants. More research is needed to determine whether these are
sample-specific find- ings or whether security associations with
maternal age and gender differences become evident during early
childhood. Gender differences have not been typically reported in
research with infants and very little information exists about them
during early childhood.
Findings from the main analyses for the entire sample and
each culture support our first hypothe- sis that sensitivity
continues to be important beyond infancy and back the idea that the
construc- tion of child security in the child–mother relation- ship
is an ongoing process that continues in early childhood. Indeed,
the information presented is consistent with the idea that, far
from being a closed issue in infancy, security outcomes remain
linked to quality of maternal care in early child- hood.
Although important, sensitivity, in all likelihood, is
manifested differently at various ages. Thus, we provide evidence
in support of behavioral domains hypothesized to be directly
relevant to preschoolers’ secure base use. Specifically, we
considered maternal contributions to and participation in
harmonious interactions with her child, secure base support (i.e.,
being there for her child when she or he wants/needs to go back,
and supporting explo-
Table 5 Partial Correlations Between Maternal Behavior for
Preschoolers Q-Set Subscales and Security Across Settings
Security
Overall sample 1. Smooth interactions .36* 2. Secure base
support .31* 3. Supervision .29* 4. Limit setting .21*
Colombia 1. Smooth interactions .35*** 2. Secure base support
.24* 3. Supervision .23* 4. Limit setting .07
Mexico 1. Smooth interactions .30* 2. Secure base support
.28* 3. Supervision .15 4. Limit setting .18
Peru 1. Smooth interactions .44** 2. Secure base support
.43** 3. Supervision .53** 4. Limit setting .28†
United States 1. Smooth interactions .19* 2. Secure base
support .19* 3. Supervision .05 4. Limit setting .17†
Note. Maternal age, education, child age, and gender
controlled for in all analyses. †p < .10. *p < .05. **p <
.01. ***p < .001, one-tail tests.
306 Posada et al.
rations away from mother), monitoring and super- vising her
child’s whereabouts, and setting limits in consideration of the
child’s wishes. For the total sample, these domains turned out to
be signifi- cantly related to preschool children’s trust in their
mother’s availability and response, that is, security. Results are
potentially important in that they target specific aspects of
mothers’ behavior when interact- ing with their preschoolers that
may help clarify the sensitivity–security link at this
developmental stage. By the same token, they encourage us to go
beyond the global label of “sensitivity” and study what caregivers
do when interacting with their chil- dren at different
developmental points.
A key hypothesis in the Bowlby–Ainsworth per- spective is the
idea that despite immense variability in maternal caregiving, the
general organization of maternal behavior during interactions with
her child, as assessed by the sensitivity construct, is related to
patterns of secure base use in different social and cultural
contexts and settings (Ains- worth, 1977; Bowlby, 1969/1982;
Posada, Carbonell, Alzate, & Plata, 2004; Posada et al., 2002;
van IJzen- doorn & Sagi-Schwartz, 2008). Our second hypothe-
sis was concerned with the generality of the sensitivity–security
link in samples from four differ- ent cultural/social groups.
First, a comparison among samples indicated that there were some
sig- nificant differences in mean levels of sensitivity and
security. Specifically, the U.S. sample had signifi- cantly higher
sensitivity scores than the other sam- ples; in addition, the
Colombian sample obtained higher scores than the sample from Peru.
Further- more, the U.S. sample obtained significantly higher
security scores than the samples from Colombia and Peru. Higher
sensitivity and security scores in the United States may be related
to the specific liv- ing conditions of the samples. Other research
has shown that both sensitivity and security scores are context
sensitive and related to the particular ecol- ogy of the dyads
studied; for example, they are lower in samples from
low-socioeconomic sectors of the population (Mesman, van
IJzendoorn, & Baker- mans-Kranenburg, 2012; Posada et al.,
1999; Zevalkink & Riksen-Walraven, 2001), as in the case of
Mexican immigrant and Peruvian dyads. As Colombian dyads are
concerned, even though they came from the middle sectors of the
population, previous research has shown that such sectors are not
directly comparable to middle-class families in the United States
(Posada et al., 2002). These results are important because they
indicate that real-life conditions impact parents’ ability to
interact sensi- tively with their child and child security.
Despite those differences, however, descriptions of maternal
behavior were significantly associated with the organization of
children’s secure base behavior in all samples. Maternal
sensitivity, derived from observations of maternal behavior
conducted in a different setting and at a different time from
observations of child behavior, turned out to be significantly
associated with child secu- rity. Thus, the association cannot be
simply con- strued as an artifact of observations conducted on both
members of the dyad in the same setting at the same time. The
partial correlation coefficients between the constructs across
settings indicate that this is not the case. Findings further
emphasize Bowlby and Ainsworth’s central ideas that it is in the
context of daily mother–child exchanges that children organize and
maintain their attachment relationships and that maternal and child
behavior are interlocked (Ainsworth et al., 1978; Bowlby,
1969/1982). Thus, results back the hypothesis that maternal
behavior indicative of awareness of the child’s signals and
communications, accurate inter- pretation of those signals, and
prompt and appro- priate responding plays a key role in the
organization of a child’s secure base behavior.
It is important to note, however, that this study was not
concerned with testing differences in how diverse cultural/social
groups implement maternal care or child secure base behavior. Those
differ- ences are likely to exist (e.g., Posada et al., 2013; van
IJzendoorn & Sagi-Schwartz, 2008), but we argue that studies
about cultural variation on these issues should state such
differences in advance and test their hypotheses with data. In our
case, we were interested in whether the general organization of
maternal behavior in interactions with her child was related to
patterns of secure base use, charac- terized as security. This was
so in every sample studied.
Additionally, we were interested in exploring specific
domains of maternal behavior that help us understand the
sensitivity–security association dur- ing early childhood. Findings
confirmed the notion that maternal contributions to harmonious
child– mother interactions and provision of secure base support are
central to secure relationships. Simi- larly, supervision and
monitoring of child activities and sensitive limit setting seem to
be potentially important factors when accounting for the relations
between sensitivity and security.
A key aspect of maternal care, as far as security outcomes
are concerned, is a mother’s ability to establish and contribute to
harmonious exchanges with her child. Much as Ainsworth emphasized
the
Sensitivity–Security Link 307
importance of maternal behavior that contributes to smooth
interactions during infancy (Ainsworth et al., 1978), we found that
mothers who contribute to smooth emotional give-and-take exchanges,
“work as a team” during interactions with their children, and
involve themselves both behaviorally and affectively had
preschool-aged children with higher security scores. Again, this
was so for the total sample and for each subsample. Maternal
contributions to harmonious exchanges remain a central ingredient
for the construction of child attachment security during the
preschool years.
Furthermore, providing secure base support is central to a
child’s construction of trust in her or his mother’s availability
and response. Being there when needed and contributing to a child’s
explo- rations are likely to build a child’s feelings of secu- rity
in her or his transactions with surroundings. Our inquiry about
mothers’ provision of a haven of safety to which to go back when
upset (e.g., when having an accident or when afraid), and a secure
base from which to explore and use to navigate his or her
surroundings (e.g., enhancing the child’s activities in ways that
make him or her feel effec- tive and pleased about transactions
with the envi- ronment) indeed confirmed the link between maternal
patterns of secure base support and secure base use in young
children. This was so for the total sample and for each of the
samples in the study. These findings support the relevance of the
secure base phenomenon as a central factor when investigating and
characterizing child–mother attachment relationships during
childhood.
Being aware of her child’s whereabouts is also essential for
a mother to respond sensitively (e.g., Ainsworth et al., 1978).
Conceptualized as maternal supervision and monitoring of her child
(Posada et al., 2007), maternal behavior that keeps track of child
activities, anticipates problematic situations, and balances the
tasks of monitoring and partici- pating in child activities was
found to be signifi- cantly associated with child security scores
for the overall sample. Similar associations were found for the
Colombian and Peruvian samples. The associa- tion was not
significant in the other two subsam- ples. This latter result may
be due to a very restricted range of scores in these subsamples; 41
of the 46 scores in the sample of Mexican immigrants ranged from 5
to 7.5, and 73 of the 76 scores in the U.S. sample ranged from 5 to
7.75 (possible range = 1–9). Indeed, descriptive analyses indicated
high kurtosis and a negatively skewed distribution in both
subsamples, which may explain why this finding is at odds with
findings for the Colombian
and Peruvian samples and previous results (Posada et al.,
2007). Further research with samples exhibit- ing more variability
in this domain is needed. As a whole, however, maternal supervision
and monitor- ing turned out to be significantly related to chil-
dren’s secure base use for the total sample.
Finally, we explored whether the way mothers go about limit
setting is related to security out- comes. It has been argued
elsewhere that in setting limits, a sensitive mother reasons with
her child, takes into account her or his wishes, and guides and
lets her child participate in decision making (e.g., Posada et al.,
2007). Results for the overall sample supported this notion.
Analyses within each subsample, however, indicated that the
constructs were not significantly related. This finding may be
attributed to the marginal internal consistency indices in each
subsample for the scale representing this domain. That is, the few
items comprising the scale may not provide a reliable estimate of
sensi- tive limit setting. Once assessment issues are addressed,
more research is warranted to determine whether the domain plays an
important role in sen- sitive caregiving during early childhood.
For the entire sample, however, support was provided for the
hypothesis that mothers who reason with their children about rules,
take into account their wishes, and guide and let their children
participate in deci- sion making when setting limits, have children
who use them as a secure base.
Importantly, there are limitations in the study presented.
First, although different cultural and social settings were
sampled, findings are limited to the regions where the subsamples
originated. That is, although the data support the hypothesis con-
cerned with the generality of the sensitivity–secu- rity link,
these findings need to be tested in cultural groups that differ
from the ones studied here, for example, countries in Africa, Asia,
and the Middle East. Furthermore, they need to be confirmed with
different samples from the countries studied here. A greater sample
size, especially in the case of the Mexican immigrant and Peruvian
dyads, is desir- able. Finally, sample characteristics and
procedures varied somewhat from country to country; similar
research protocols would facilitate comparisons across cultural
groups. Recruitment of samples, per- sonnel, time, and finances,
however, are important constraints to address.
Despite some differences in sample size and length and number
of observations across samples, the data gathered supported the
main hypotheses and speak to the importance of the relations
between sensitivity and security during early
308 Posada et al.
childhood in different cultural and social settings. Findings
highlight the relevance of investigating child–mother attachment
relationships from dyadic and behavioral perspectives during early
child- hood (Bretherton & Munholland, 2008; Posada & Lu,
2011). An interperson approach acknowledges that attachment
phenomena not only influence but also are influenced by
interpersonal exchanges. Certainly, from a developmental point of
view, it is highly likely that secure base relationships are in the
process of being elaborated and consoli- dated during childhood.
Attention to maternal (caregiver’s) contributions and relationship
experi- ences may clarify our understanding of the devel- opment of
attachment relationships after infancy. A more fine-grain analysis
of maternal behavior is necessary to include both general
descriptions of constructs and more specific instances of how those
constructs are observable during actual interactions. In doing so,
the need for construct fidelity is an important consideration (De
Wolff & van IJzendoorn, 1997). We also consider it essen- tial
to approach the issues at hand from a multi- contextual
perspective, with specific hypotheses about similarities and
differences across social and cultural contexts specified
beforehand and tested empirically. In accomplishing this task, the
use of diverse approaches, for example, ethnographies, may be
necessary to dispel possible methodologi- cal confounds. Finally,
testing the universality of core hypotheses within attachment
theory does not necessarily imply the use of samples from dif-
ferent countries, but could also accommodate the use of within
culture/country groups (as in the case of immigrant Mexican dyads
in the present study).
In sum, observations of maternal and child behavior during
child–mother interactions at home and playgrounds in four samples
from different contexts were used to test the hypothesis that
maternal sensitivity is related to child attachment security in
early childhood across diverse cultures. Results indicated that for
the total sample as well as for each of the four groups, the
constructs are significantly associated. Mothers who are sensi-
tively responsive to their preschoolers’ communica- tions and
behavior tend to have children whose behavior indicates trust in
their mother’s availabil- ity and response. These findings support
the notions that child–mother attachment relationships continue to
be constructed during childhood and that this phenomenon is
commonplace in different cultural contexts.
References
Ainsworth, M. D. (1977). Attachment theory and its util- ity
in cross-cultural research. In P. H. Leiderman, S. R. Tulkin, &
A. Rosenfeld (Eds.), Culture and infancy: Vari- ations in the human
experience (pp. 49–67). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1991). Attachment and other affec-
tional bonds across the life cycle. In C. M. Parkes, J.
Stevenson-Hinde, & P. Marris (Eds.), Attachment across the life
cycle (pp. 33–51). London, UK: Routledge.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Bell, S., & Stayton, D. (1971).
Indi- vidual differences in strange situation behavior of one-
year-olds. In H. Schaeffer (Ed.), The origins of human social
relations (pp. 17–52). London, UK: Academic Press.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Bell, S. M., & Stayton, D. J.
(1974). Infant-mother attachment and social development. In M. P.
M. Richards (Ed.), The integration of a child into a social world
(pp. 99–136). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall,
S. (1978). Patterns of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., &
Juffer, F. (2003). Less is more: Meta-analyses of sensitiv- ity and
attachment interventions in early childhood. Psychological
Bulletin, 129, 195–215. doi:10.1037/0033- 2909.129.2.195
Barnett, D., Kidwell, S. L., & Leung, K. H. (1998).
Parent- ing and preschooler attachment among low-income urban
African American families. Child Development, 69, 1657–1671.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06183.x
Block, J. (1978). The Q-sort method. Palo Alto, CA: Consult-
ing Psychologists Press.
Bowlby, J. (1958). The nature of the child’s tie to his
mother. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 39, 350–373.
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment.
New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent–child attachment and
healthy human development. London, UK: Basic Books (Original work
published 1969).
Bretherton, I., & Munholland, K. A. (2008). Internal
work- ing models in attachment relationships: Elaborating a central
construct in attachment theory. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver
(Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical
applications (2nd ed., pp. 102–127). New York, NY: Guilford.
De Wolff, M., & van IJzendoorn, M. (1997). Sensitivity
and attachment: A meta-analysis on parental antece- dents of infant
attachment. Child Development, 68, 571– 591.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb04218.x
George, C., & Solomon, J. (2008). The caregiving system:
A behavioral systems approach to parenting. In J. Cassidy & P.
R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and
clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 833–856). New York, NY:
Guilford.
Kermoian, R., & Leiderman, P. H. (1986). Infant attach-
ment to mother and child caretaker in a east African
Sensitivity–Security Link 309
https://allaplusessays.com/order. International Journal of
Behavioral Develop- ment, 9, 455–469.
doi:10.1177/016502548600900404
Leadbeater, B. J., & Bishop, S. J. (1994). Predictors of
behavior problems in preschool children of inner-city Afro-American
and Puerto Rican adolescent mothers. Child Development, 65,
638–648. doi:10.1111/j.1467- 8624.1994.tb00773.x
LeVine, R. A., & Norman, K. (2001). The infant’s acquisi-
tion of culture: Early attachment reexamined in anthro- pological
perspective. In C. C. Moore & H. F. Mathews (Eds.), The
psychology of cultural experience (pp. 83–104). Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Leyendecker, B., & Lamb, M. E. (1999). Latino families.
In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), Parenting and child development in “nontradi-
tional” families (pp. 247–262). New York, NY: Erlbaum.
Leyendecker, B., Lamb, M. E., Scholmerich, A., & Fra-
casso, M. P. (1995). The social worlds of 8- and 12- month-old
infants: Early experiences in two subcultural contexts. Social
Development, 4, 194–208. doi:10.1111/ j.1467-9507.1995.tb00060.x
Martin, C. A., & Colbert, K. K. (1997). Parenting: A life
span perspective. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Marvin, R. S., & Britner, P. A. (1999). Normative
develop- ment: The ontogeny of attachment. In J. Cassidy & P.
R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and
clinical applications (pp. 44–67). New York, NY: Guilford.
Mesman, J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranen-
burg, M. J. (2012). Unequal in opportunity, equal in process:
Parental sensitivity promotes positive child development in ethnic
minority families. Child Develop- ment Perspectives, 6, 239–250.
doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606. 2011.00223.x
Nakagawa, M., Lamb, M. E., & Miyaki, K. (1992). An-
tecedents and correlates of the Strange Situation behav- ior of
Japanese infants. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 23,
300–310. doi:10.1177/0022022192233002
Okagaki, L., & Johnson-Divecha, D. (1993). Development of
parental beliefs. In T. Luster & L. Okagaki (Eds.), Parenting:
An ecological perspective (pp. 35–67). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002).
Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of
theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psycho- logical
Bulletin, 128, 3–72. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.128.1.3
Pederson, D. R., & Moran, G. (1996). Expressions of the
attachment relationship outside of the Strange Situa- tion. Child
Development, 67, 915–927. doi:10.1111/j.1467- 8624.1996.tb01773.x
Pianta, R. C., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (1989).
Continu- ity and discontinuity in maternal sensitivity at 6, 24,
and 42 months in a high-risk sample. Child Develop- ment, 60,
481–487. doi:10.2307/1130992
Posada, G., Carbonell, O. A., Alzate, G., & Plata, S. J.
(2004). Through Colombian lenses: Ethnographic and conventional
analyses of maternal care and their associ- ations with secure base
behavior. Developmental Psychol- ogy, 40, 508–518.
doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.4.508
Posada, G., Gao, Y., Wu, F., Posada, R., Tascon, M.,
Sch€oelmerich, A., . . . Synnevaag, B. (1995). The secure- base
phenomenon across cultures: Children’s behavior, mothers’
preferences, and experts’ concepts. Child Development, 60, 27–48.
doi:10.1111/j.1540-5834.1995. tb00202.x
Posada, G., Jacobs, A., Carbonell, O. A., Alzate, G., Busta-
mante, M. R., & Arenas, A. (1999). Maternal care and attachment
security in ordinary and emergency con- texts. Developmental
Psychology, 35, 1379–1388. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.35.6.1379
Posada, G., Jacobs, A., Richmond, M. K., Carbonell, O. A.,
Alzate, G., Bustamante, M. R., & Quiceno, H. (2002). Maternal
caregiving and infant security in two cultures. Developmental
Psychology, 38, 67–78. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.38.1.67
Posada, G., Kaloustian, G., Richmond, M. K., & Moreno, A.
J. (2007). Maternal secure base support and preschoolers’ secure
base behavior in natural environ- ments. Attachment & Human
Development, 9, 393–411. doi:10.1080/14616730701712316
Posada, G., & Lu, T. (2011). Child-parent attachment
rela- tionships: A life-span phenomenon. In K. L. Fingerman, C. A.
Berg, J. Smith, & T. C. Antonucci (Eds.), Handbook of life-span
development (pp. 87–115). New York, NY: Springer.
Posada, G., Lu, T., Trumbell, J., Kaloustian, G., Trudel, M.,
Plata, S., . . . Lay, K. L. (2013). Is the secure base phenomenon
evident here, there, and anywhere? A cross-cultural study of child
behavior and experts’ defi- nitions. Child Development, 84,
1896–1905. doi:10.1111/ cdev.12084
Richmond, M., Posada, G., & Jacobs, A. (2001, April). Ma-
ternal behavior and attachment security in 3- year-olds: A
naturalistic study. Poster presented at the meeting of the Society
for Research in Child Development, Min- neapolis, MN.
Rivera, F. I., Guarnaccia, P. J., Mulvaney-Day, N., Lin, J.
Y., Torres, M., & Alegria, M. (2008). Family cohesion and its
relationship to psychological distress among Latino groups.
Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 30, 357–378.
doi:10.1177/0739986308318713
Rothbaum, F., & Morelli, G. (2005). Attachment and cul-
ture: Bridging relativism and universalism. In W. Friedlmeier, P.
Chakkarath, & B. Schwarz (Eds.), Cul- ture and human
development: The importance of cross-cul- tural research for the
social sciences (pp. 99–123). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Rothbaum, F., Weisz, J., Pott, M., Miyake, K., & Morelli,
G. (2001). Deeper into attachment and culture. American
Psychologist, 56, 827–829. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.56. 10.827
Rudy, D., & Grusec, J. E. (2006). Authoritarian parenting
in individualist and collectivist groups: Associations with
maternal emotion and cognition and children’s self-esteem. Journal
of Family Psychology, 20, 68–78. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.20.1.68
310 Posada et al.
https://allaplusessays.com/order, L. A. (1988). The role of
infant-caregiver attach- ment in development. In J. Belsky & T.
Nezworski (Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment (pp. 18–38).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Sroufe, L. A. (2002). From infant attachment to promotion of
adolescent autonomy: Prospective, longitudinal data on the role of
parents in development. In J. G. Bor- kowski, S. L. Ramey & M.
Bristol-Power (Eds.), Parent- ing and the child’s world: Influences
on academic, intellectual, and social-emotional development.
Monographs in Parenting (pp. 187–202). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Stevenson-Hinde, J., & Shouldice, A. (1995). Maternal
interactions and self-reports related to attachment clas-
sifications at 4.5 years. Child Development, 66, 583–596.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00891.x
Valenzuela, M. (1997). Maternal sensitivity in a develop- ing
society: The context of urban poverty and infant chronic
undernutrition. Developmental Psychology, 33, 845–855.
doi:10.1037/0012-1649.33.5.845
van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Sagi-Schwartz, A. (2008). Cross-
cultural patterns of attachment: Universal and contex- tual
dimensions. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of
attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd ed.,
pp. 880–905). New York, NY: Guilford.
van IJzendoorn, M. H., Vereijken, C. M. J. L., Bakermans-
Kranenburg, M. J., & Riksen-Walraven, J. M. (2004). Assessing
attachment security with the Attachment Q Sort: Meta-analytic
evidence for the validity of the observer AQS. Child Development,
75, 1188–1213. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00733.x
Vaughn, B. E., & Waters, E. (1990). Attachment behavior
at home and in the laboratory: Q-sort observations and strange
situation classifications of one-year-olds. Child
Development, 61, 1965–1973. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
8624.1990.tb03578.x
Vereijken, C. M. J. L., Riksen-Walraven, J. M., & Kondo-
Ikemura, K. (1997). Maternal sensitivity and infant attachment
security in Japan: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology,
21, 35–49. doi:10.1080/ 016502597384974
Waters, E. (1995). Appendix A: The Attachment Q-Set (version
3.0). Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development,
60(Serial No. 224), 234–246. doi:10.2307/1166181
Waters, E., & Cummings, E. M. (2000). A secure base from
which to explore close relationships. Child Devel- opment, 71,
164–172. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00130
Waters, E., Kondo-Ikemura, K., Posada, G., & Richters, J.
E. (1991). Learning to love: Milestones and mecha- nisms. In M. R.
Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe (Eds.), Self pro- cesses and development:
The Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 217–255).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Waters, E., Posada, G., Crowell, J. A., & Lay, K. L.
(1994). The development of attachment: From control system to
working models. Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biologi- cal
Processes, 57, 32–42. doi: 10.1521/00332747. 1994.11024666
Zevalkink, J., & Riksen-Walraven, J. M. (2001). Parenting
in Indonesia: Inter- and intracultural differences in mothers’
interactions with their young children. Interna- tional Journal of
Behavioral Development, 25, 167–175. doi:10.1080/01650250042000113
Zuniga, M. E. (1992). Families with Latino roots. In E. W.
Lynch & M. J. Hanson (Eds.), Developing cross-cultural
competence: A guide for working with young children and their
families (pp. 151–179). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
Sensitivity–Security Link 311
https://allaplusessays.com/order
The post The Sensitivity–Security Link in Early Childhood
appeared first on Lion Essays.
“Looking for a Similar Assignment? Get Expert Help at an
Amazing Discount!”
The Sensitivity–Security Link in Early Childhood was first posted on February 28, 2019 at 9:09 pm.©2019 “Lion Essays”. Use of this feed is for personal non-commercial use only. If you are not reading this article in your feed reader, then the site is guilty of copyright infringement. Please contact me at [email protected] Essays.com
"Is this question part of your assignment? We Can Help!"












Other samples, services and questions:
When you use PaperHelp, you save one valuable — TIME
You can spend it for more important things than paper writing.